The Result of University Cost-Cutting Measures . . .
the Plausible Deniability Blog takes up where the PostModernVillage blog left off. While you'll see many of the same names here, PDB allows its writers and editors a space away from financial strum und drang that torpedoed the PMV blog.
Wednesday, August 28, 2013
First lines from a failed novel: "Tabitha was suffering from the deep exhaustion that comes from always having to be right. As she stared at her ice cream, whiteness melting into the china, she wondered what her life would be like if she was the sort of person who could be satisfied by an evening at Applebee's."
Thursday, August 22, 2013
Wednesday, August 21, 2013
Sunday, July 14, 2013
Social Biases and the Follies of Science
By EW Wilder
Sometimes, science is simply looking in
the wrong place.
If we accept the notion that scientific
inquiry itself is a social phenomenon, then it becomes easy to see
why certain things are studied while others are not. The male gender
bias in studies of heart disease are legendary, so I won't go deeply
into them here, but they should have been cautionary: any time
research money flows, the question that should be asked is “What
cultural biases are inherent to the request?”
In mental health, one of those biases
is in favor of a strictly biological model; that is, if your woes are
experiential, they are necessarily a problem with your brain.
If you reel at what seems to be such a
broad-sweeping generalization on my part, witness a recent report
on NPR on some current efforts to use biomarkers to determine if a
person is mentally ill. (Listen to this one, as the written synopsis
does not give the full report.) The part about PTSD is the most
illustrative, with researchers pouring millions into brain scans and
computer programs that are supposed to “detect” PTSD. That the
military is a large part of this research isn't surprising, but it
should be: the people they're studying were in combat after
all.
Which goes back to my point: if you
have a combat veteran who is complaining about being unable to sleep,
feeling constantly sad, being unable to concentrate, and flying into
sudden rages, you could spend millions trying to figure out what was
wrong. Or you could just ask him.
Our culture somehow discounts the idea
that somebody's story of trauma and that the disruption to their
lives that this can lead to are “real” (even though we frequently
see the effects directly) unless and until there is some “objective”
measure: a biomarker, a study, a brain scan.
There are two phenomena at work, and
they represent the major streams of American life. One is the
rationalist tradition that hies from the Age of Reason. This
movement, in many ways, created our nation; the so-called “Founding
Fathers” were rationalists, many of them deists, and even such
notions as the idea that free people ought to be governed by bodies
in which occurred reasoned debate (instead of by the caprices of a
king) is a testament to this fact.
Rationalism is great; it has brought us
the computers you're reading this on. It brought us modern sanitation
and antibiotics and refrigeration. It made sense out of a seeable
universe that just seemed to be jacking with us heretofore. But it
also has a bad habit of blinding us to the more obvious cultural
constants that create our lives: maybe the people who “have PTSD”
have simply learned to adapt to an impossibly violent and stressful
environment, and maybe those adaptations are ill-matched to a life of
cheeseburgers and traffic lights. Maybe those who are labeled
“bipolar” have lost their sense of balance because they have had
to adapt to unbalanced lives. Maybe depressed people have damn good
reasons to be very, very sad.
But the fact that these adaptations
have profound effects on the lives of those who experience them and
that those effects impact the abilities of people to meet the
expectations of our society mean that they have a moral component,
not just a medical one. This is where the other major stream of
American life comes into play: Puritanism.
No matter what our official religious
affiliation may be, even if we have none, American culture still is
Puritan in the sense that it associates productivity with value; if
you don't work, you're still stigmatized, particularly if your
ailment isn't readily visible. That many employers view being
unemployed as, itself, a strike against a job applicant shows how
deep this cultural assumption is. Further, your engagement with work
must always be a happy one: management books are all about how to
reinforce upon the workforce how wonderful it is to be at work, even
if your entire life seems to be crumbling around you. The reason
managers insist that their employees all read Who Moved My Cheese?
and The 7 Habits of Highly Effective People is that they
believe that employees have an obligation, as an expectation of their
employment, to always adapt and always produce. There is no room for
(and considerable opprobrium in) merely being human.
This also helps explain why, despite
the protestations of psychiatrists, including a prominent one in the
NPR report, the
disease model of mental illness actually increases stigma,
rather than reducing it. If your problems have to do with your life,
that's something you can change, but if your problems have to do with
your brain, you are, for the moral purposes of a nation that
associates productivity with being a good person, damaged goods.
My main problem with the search for
biomarkers when it comes to those who go through extreme
psychological states is not that it's a waste of money and time. If
they are doing basic research, it's well worth doing. The
brain's job is to adapt to and learn from the environment, and I'm
sure they could find all sorts of interesting things about how the
brain does that.
My problem is that they will miss the
problems that underly the causes of the brain's adaptations to these
states: a culture that does not merely countenance, but often
reinforces psychological damage, that fails to address the social
inequities and poor policies--both foreign and domestic--that
perpetuate violence and despair, and that prevents healing by blaming
the suffering and keeping in place schools, social spaces, and
workplaces that are essentially inhumane.
We would rather spend millions over and
over again to keep parsing out the biological details than spend the
billions once to solve what we already know in our hearts, but will
not admit in our heads, ails us.
Thursday, July 4, 2013
The Opposite of What You Know
EW Wilder
The more we see of the world, the more
obvious it becomes: power protects itself.
This is evident from the smallest of
power differentials—those between teacher and student, doctor and
patient, worker and direct supervisor—all the way up to national
and international power relationships. Because of this, it becomes
the role of those in positions of power to protect that power first
and foremost: the actual services or activities institutions with
managerial power structures ostensibly exist to support are secondary
at best; often they are almost an afterthought.
Supervisory roles, with their
performance reviews and managerial expectations, are designed not to
serve those directly performing the work nor to help the processes of
production, service provision, or exchange. Rather, they exist to
hold the worker “accountable” not to the ostensible effort of the
organization, but to the managerial structure itself. This shifts
responsibility from becoming concentrated up the supervisory train,
as remuneration and role would suggest it should do, but away from it
and onto the least compensated, least powerful members of the
organization.
The protestations of those at the top
that they deserve extra pay because they take extra risks and have
greater responsibilities simply aren't true: those at the top are
more able to manage their own time, more likely to have close
relationships with those who supervise them (typically a board of
directors for publicly run companies and non-profits), and have the
clout to negotiate huge salaries and severance packages. This helps
create conditions for them to be irresponsible with impunity, which
would be impossible for low-level employees who are subject to many
levels of supervision, demeaning if not infantilizing policies and
procedures, and performance reviews designed to blame them when
anything goes wrong.
Note, also, that the supposedly “free”
market backs up existing power structures as well, demanding layoffs
first when a company loses money, not that the CEO be fired. This is
the opposite of what would be demanded of a college sports team, for
example, or even a politician.
Managerial structures that reinforce
power at the top create relationships with workers based on the
negative: they kick into gear when the worker does wrong; they are
punitive of her errors, not supportive of her efforts. Control over
her own work, much less initiative and innovation, are a threat to
power and therefore must be punished, no matter how useful they may
be. And so a worker's role is strictly defined by procedure and
policy. Further, the supporting structures that do exist are taken
out of the control of the restive worker and insourced to separate
“administrative” or “operations” departments, departments
subject to punitive power structures of their own.
This concentrates the power of those at
the top even more, pitting departments against each other for control
over the resources necessary to get the work done and making the
worker constantly under threat from that competition: if a department
that is also competing for the company's resources keeps those
resources from a worker, she will still be blamed for not getting the
job done and subsequently punished. The power structure wins by a
divide and conquer strategy. Departments, also, have no other
recourse than to rely on the managerial power structure to work out
the inevitable (and designed-in) problems between each other,
reinforcing its power yet again and preventing worker
collectivization.
If we see existing managerial power
structures for what they really are, ways to protect those in charge
and not ways to get work done, we may begin to address problems not
only of inequality but also of inefficiency. But because we believe
so deeply in these structures and replicate them everywhere from
schools to the executive branch of government, change is unlikely to
happen. And it's unlikely that those in power would let it happen:
already we see the incredible influence of those industrialists and
executives who would seek to impose this structure on our
representative bodies themselves by electing “free market”
politicians, whose stated goals are to bring these principles to the
public sphere. Again, the rhetoric is the opposite of the intention,
as we have seen how these politicians act when they get into office:
in Kansas, and Texas, Wisconsin and Ohio, they have acted like petty
dictators, bullying and dominating wherever they can, ignoring both
constitutionality and the financial stability, much less best
interests of, the states they serve.
The goal of these people is to usurp
what little democracy our republic allows and decimate our last, best
hope at retaining the popular rule we have so willingly given up in
the workplaces that, if we are lucky enough to be employed, already
dominate our lives.
Sunday, June 23, 2013
The Trouble with Contemporary Literature is a Matter of Voice
by Lael Ewy
An example of contemporary literature's
maddening problems with voice is Charles Baxter's short story
“Loyalty” which appeared in the May 2013 issue of Harper's.
I like both Harper's and Baxter's poetry, and that makes this
story all the more maddening. The voice in the story is not
completely unbelievable, it's just enough “off” to create both
tonal and cognitive dissonances that get in the way of enjoying the
story and appreciating its thematic attempt.
For example, the narrator, ostensibly a
mechanic, delivers lines such as these: “Love for Astrid like a
climbing vine grew out of my heart. I don't know how else to say it”
(77). But here's the thing: just about any mechanic would know
how else to say it, in lots of ways, not the least of which would be
“My love for Astrid was like a vine growing out of my heart.” As
it appears in Baxter's story this is a line of poetry written by
Charles Baxter, not a statement about love as spoken or thought by a
mechanic. As I reformulated it here, it's more like a country song,
as it should be: vastly more mechanics listen to country music than
study creative writing. That's not stereotyping; it's a simple matter
or statistics, and if by losing his lovely line Baxter gains some
authenticity, his story is all the better for it.
And there's more: “He looks past me
as if I were a footnote” (78) expresses the narrator's reaction to
his teenage son, as if a footnote were the first thing that would
come to a mechanic's mind when he was searching for a way to explain
what it felt like to be ignored. “He looked right through me” or
“He looked at me like I was thin air” are both dangerously
clichéd, but they're a hell of a lot more true to the way most
mechanics I know would think. Or take something like “She stands
audibly” (78) which, for as simple as it is, is also not what a
mechanic would say. He'd understand the physics of it, for one thing,
and that would occur to him. What about “You could hear the cheap
cushions suck in as she stood up”? Not as lovely, but a crap-ton
more true.
Or take “I feel an antiquated tingle”
as the narrator describes his feeling when he is kissed by his
ex-wife (78). An Antiquated Tingle might be a great title for
an alt country album, but a mechanic would say “I felt the old
tingle,“ which he'd then joke about with his buddies down at the
shop (and our narrator appears to have none) riffing on how he's “got
your 'old tingle' riiight here!” It may seem crass, and it would
make the story veer in a direction Baxter may not have been prepared
for, but that's what would have happened.
Other touches seem minor, but they are
glaring. In an attempt at authenticity, Baxter has his narrator
notice how “the front end dipped from the bad shocks” (76).
That's a piece of writing that has an assonance perfectly balanced,
with a subtle interplay between that assonance and the consonants in
the line that could take up 15 minutes or more of discussion in a
literature class. It also isn't true. Bad shocks make a car bounce,
not dip, as any mechanic worth his salt would know. At one point,
Baxter's narrator's eyes glance over a “corroded timing light”
(76) which, sure, he would notice, but doesn't bother to make
note of the vintage of his own F-150 pickup with its “loose fan
belt” (79). That's significant because no modern manufacturer uses
separate fan belts anymore (they use a single “serpentine belt”
setup for all the engine accessories), and they haven't for 20-some
years. An F-150 with a fan belt would be approaching classic status,
so it would be a real shame to let it rust (76), something our
supposed mechanic of a narrator would doubtlessly know, and which
would have potentially provided some rich thematic overtones to what
happens to the narrator's ex-wife as she spirals into mental anguish,
had Baxter been aware of what a mechanic would have been aware of.
Throughout the story, Baxter can't seem
to decide if his narrator worked at a small, independent “shop”
(78) or the service department of a dealership or department store
that would have a separate “Parts Department” (81). This is no
small difference, as working in a small shop might not be as
lucrative as working at a dealership (where trained mechanics can
earn upwards of $50 an hour and receive decent benefits). The
narrator's lifestyle appears to be middle class, suggesting a
dealership, but if that's the case, why hasn't he also bought himself
a nicer truck?
To his credit, Baxter does create a
reasonably intelligent narrator, and successful mechanics, unlike the
“grease monkey” stereotype, have got to be intelligent people.
This shows, perhaps, the problem with voice in contemporary
literature: it is written almost exclusively by people whose
experiences are limited to the academic world and the middle-class
childhoods that create academics. These writers want to tell the
stories of people who are not like them. They need to maintain the
proper mildly liberal political correctness about those who are not
like them. But these writers also need to show off the depth of their
word-craft to editors who come from the same middle-class and
academic backgrounds. Combined, all this leads to a lot of short
stories just like Baxter's “Loyalty”: narrators who all sound
like they have MFAs in creative writing peppering their narratives
with little bits of “authenticity” that speak much more to the
attempts of the writer than to anything about the world the
narrators—or, for that matter, the rest of us—actually live in.
In that world, mechanics do speak a
certain poetry. But the sort of poetry they speak is spoken
accidentally, and to catch it requires listening to real
mechanics, listing deeply and listening well, and listening for a
long time. And sure, some mechanics have MFAs in creative writing
themselves and may very well express themselves as Baxter presents
here. But the narrator of “Loyalty” never suggests this about
himself, and anyway, why that person would be a mechanic would
necessarily comprise the bulk of the story, and that's not what
Baxter seems to want to explore.
What he does explore has merit; the
kind of relational ambiguity and family dynamics he presents do ring
true to life.
Too bad the rest of it doesn't.
Work Cited
Baxter, Charles. “Loyalty.”
Harper's Magazine. May 2013: 76-82. Print.
Wednesday, June 12, 2013
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)